Male and female bees show large differences in floral preference (2024)

As a library, NLM provides access to scientific literature. Inclusion in an NLM database does not imply endorsem*nt of, or agreement with, the contents by NLM or the National Institutes of Health.
Learn more: PMC Disclaimer | PMC Copyright Notice

Male and female bees show large differences in floral preference (1)

Link to Publisher's site

PLoS One. 2019; 14(4): e0214909.

Published online 2019 Apr 24. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0214909

PMCID: PMC6481915

PMID: 31017928

Michael Roswell, Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing,1,2,* Jonathan Dushoff, Formal analysis, Methodology, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing,3 and Rachael Winfree, Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing2

Manu E. Saunders, Editor

Author information Article notes Copyright and License information PMC Disclaimer

This article has been corrected. See PLoS One. 2019 June 4; 14(6): e0217714.

Associated Data

Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement

Abstract

Background

Intraspecific variation in foraging niche can drive food web dynamics and ecosystem processes. In particular, male and female animals can exhibit different, often cascading, impacts on their interaction partners. Despite this, studies of plant-pollinator interaction networks have focused on the partitioning of the floral community between pollinator species, with little attention paid to intraspecific variation in plant preference between male and female bees. We designed a field study to evaluate the strength and prevalence of sexually dimorphic foraging, and particularly resource preferences, in bees.

Study design

We observed bees visiting flowers in semi-natural meadows in New Jersey, USA. To detect differences in flower use against a shared background of resource (flower) availability, we maximized the number of interactions observed within narrow spatio-temporal windows. To distinguish observed differences in bee use of flower species, which can reflect abundance patterns and sampling effects, from underlying differences in bee preferences, we analyzed our data with both a permutation-based null model and random effects models.

Findings

We found that the diets of male and female bees of the same species were often dissimilar as the diets of different species of bees. Furthermore, we demonstrate differences in preference between male and female bees. We show that intraspecific differences in preference can be robustly identified among hundreds of unique species-species interactions, without precisely quantifying resource availability, and despite high phenological turnover of both bees and plant bloom. Given the large differences in both flower use and preferences between male and female bees, ecological sex differences should be integrated into studies of bee demography, plant pollination, and coevolutionary relationships between flowers and insects.

Introduction

Intraspecific variation in traits and behavior, including foraging niche, has important consequences for species interactions and conservation [1,2]. Sexual dimorphism is a large source of individual niche variation, and an important factor in plant-animal interactions, such as seed dispersal [3]. Sexual dimorphism underlies adaptation, speciation, and the way in which animals exploit their ecological niche [4,5]. Morphological, behavioral, and life-history dimorphisms can also drive the form and function of ecosystems, for example when predator sex ratio drives the community composition of lower trophic levels, affecting the physical and chemical properties of the environment [6,7].

Though ecological dimorphisms were first studied in vertebrates [8], they are common across taxa, including insects [9]. Surprisingly, in bees (Hymenoptera, Apoidea) for which both foraging [10] and sexual dimorphism [11] have been well studied, sexually dimorphic foraging has rarely been documented. Intraspecific variation in floral preference is known for social [12] and to a lesser extent, solitary bees [13,14], yet most community-level studies focus on species-level interactions, and specifically on how female bees forage.

Male bees differ from their better-studied female counterparts in their life history and ecology. Female bees construct, maintain, provision, and defend nests, whereas male bees primarily seek mates [15]. Both sexes drink floral nectar for their own caloric needs, but only females collect pollen to provision young, and thus forage at greater rates. Pollens from different flower species (the term we use throughout for the flowers from a species of plant) tend to be distinct not only in morphology but also in terms of nutritional content, and both of these (factors drive plant-specific foraging by bees [16]. While recent work shows variation in nectar is more important than previously acknowledged [17], even species of bees that specialize narrowly on pollen hosts (oligolects) tend to nectar from many flower species. It is unclear what factors drive male bee preferences, though the criteria males use to select floral partners probably differ from those used by females.

Although female bees are more prolific pollinators due to the greater time they spend foraging at flowers, when male bees have been studied, they prove to be important pollinators as well. This is true not only in specialized oil- or scent-collecting pollination systems, where males would be predicted to be important [1820], but also for males simply foraging for nectar [2123]. Male bees may also be particularly relevant for bee conservation. Males may be limiting in declining populations, either because genetic diversity is necessary for the development of female offspring as a result of complementary sex determination, or because mate or sperm limitation results from poor male condition [2426]. As the dispersing sex in most bee species, males may be crucial for gene flow and metapopulation persistence even when they are not locally limiting [27,28].

Observed differences in resource use, which reflects the overlap of consumers and resources (availability) as well as consumer preference, may fail to reveal more essential differences in foraging niche. Preferences may be more important than use alone in the context of species conservation, and may mediate the strength of selection imposed by interaction partners. Preference—the use of a resource in excess of its relative availability—is challenging to measure, because both resource use and availability must be known. Floral resource availability for pollinators is particularly hard to quantify outside an experimental context because the appropriate scale and units of floral resource availability are unclear. The composition, amount, and supply rate of pollen and nectar per flower, the number of flowers per inflorescence, of inflorescences per individual, and the number and distribution of individual plants over the square kilometers of a bee’s foraging range are all important components of availability [29]. Furthermore, floral availability can change rapidly over time. However, differences in flower use between bees foraging at the same place and time indicate differences in preference, which may occur between species, or between individuals of the same species.

In this study, we assess differences between floral preferences of male and female bees in the field. We collected bees foraging on flowers in meadows in New Jersey, USA. In order to observe preference differences, we collected as many individuals as possible during replicated, short (3-day) windows, during which we assumed floral availability and bee abundance were constant at each site. We compare the species composition of flowers visited by males and females of the most common bee species across the entire study as a naïve measure of differences in preference between the sexes. Then, using random effects models, we assess when differential flower species use by male and female bees likely arises from sex-specific floral preference, as opposed to shifting overlap between foragers and floral resources (i.e. changes in availability without differences in preference). Specifically, we ask

  1. How much do male and female bee diets overlap?

  2. To what degree are particular flower species disproportionately visited by bees of one sex?

  3. To what extent are differences in floral use driven by preference, rather than phenological differences between male and female bees?

Materials and methods

Study design and data collection

Because absolute preference is nearly impossible to observe outside of an experiment, we designed our study to reveal differences in preference between groups of bees. In order to collect a large number of males and females from many native bee species, we selected six meadows (sites) in New Jersey, USA with a high abundance and diversity of flowers. These semi-natural meadows were managed for pollinator-attractive, summer-blooming forbs through seed addition, and a combination of mowing, burning, and weed removal. Most flower species present in the meadows are native to the eastern United States. We collected our data during peak bloom and maximum day length (6 June to 20 August, 2016), and during good weather (sunny enough for observers to see their own shadow, no precipitation). We visited each site for three consecutive good weather days over five evenly spaced sampling rounds in the 11-week period of our study. In all analyses, we assume that bees and flowers detected at a site within one 3-day sampling round co-occurred. In contrast, we assume that turnover of both plant species in bloom and bee species activity can occur in the ~10 days between sampling rounds.

During each 3-day sampling round, an observer walked parallel transects through the meadow (which ranged in size from 0.8–2.2 ha; mean = 1.4 ha), observing every open flower within a moving 1-m semicircle, and net-collecting any bee seen actively foraging, which we defined as contacting anthers or collecting nectar from a flower (Figure A in S1 File). We collected all bee species except Apis mellifera L., the domesticated western honey bee, because Apis males do not forage. Observations began as soon as pollinator activity picked up in the morning (7–9 am) and continued into the late afternoon or evening until pollinator activity slowed substantially. Observers sampled nearly continuously, in 30-minute timed collection bouts with short breaks in between. If inclement weather precluded a minimum of six 30-minute sampling bouts in a day, we added an additional day to the sampling round as soon as weather permitted.

Flower species were identified in the field by the data collector. Bee species were identified using a dissecting microscope and published keys; Jason Gibbs (University of Manitoba), Joel Gardner (University of Manitoba), and Sam Droege (USGS) assisted with identification for bees in the genera Andrena, Anthophora, Coelioxys, Halictus, Heriades, Hoplitis, Hylaeus, Lasioglossum, Megachile, Melissodes, Nomada, Osmia, Pseudoanthidium, Ptilothrix, Sphecodes, Stelis, and Triepeolus, and at least one of them confirmed voucher specimens for every species. We determined every specimen to species except for the following four complexes: Most bees in the genus Nomada with bidentate mandibles (ruficornis group) were treated as one species. All specimens from the Hylaeus species complex that includes Hylaeus affinis, H. modestus, and at least one additional species, informally dubbed “species A, were treated as a single species, denoted Hylaeus affinis-modestus, because females cannot be reliably distinguished. There is a cryptic species in the genus Halictus unlikely to occur in our area, Halictus poeyi, which is not morphologically distinct from H. ligatus; we treat all specimens in this complex as Halictus ligatus. We could not confidently separate all specimens of the two closely related Lasioglossum species Lasioglossum hitchensi and L. weemsi. Thus, we treat all specimens from either species as one, denoted Lasioglossum hitchensi-weemsi.

All bee specimens are curated in the Winfree lab collection at Rutgers University, and the data used in this paper, along with R scripts used in data analysis and figure preparation, are available from the Dryad Digital Repository (doi:10.5061/dryad.c3rr6q1). No specific permits were required to collect these data, however, we obtained permission to access meadow habitats and sample insects from Mercer County, the Institute for Advanced Study, Somerset County, and the Raritan Headwaters Association

Analytical methods

We performed all statistical analyses and simulations using R 3.5.1 [30].

1) How much do male and female bee diets overlap?

To compare the diets of male and female bees, we used the Morisita-Horn index of resource overlap [31,32]. This dissimilarity index compares the proportion of all female bees found on each flower species to the proportion of all male bees found on each flower species. In other words, it compares the contribution of each flower species to female diets (where this term includes the food that females collect for themselves and also to feed to young) to the contribution of the same flower species to male diets. The Morisita-Horn index ranges from zero (completely similar) to one (maximally dissimilar), and has several good properties for our purposes. First, it uses proportions, placing visits from male and female bees on the same scale, even though most visits come from females. Second, it is much more sensitive to large proportions than to small ones, thereby down-weighting the contribution of flower species for which we have little information. Third, the Morisita-Horn estimates are resilient to undersampling and uneven sample size between groups [33].

To determine whether the male-female differences we observed exceeded those expected by chance, we compared the observed compositional dissimilarity between flower visits from male and from female bees to dissimilarity measures from a null model that randomly permuted the bee sex associated with each flower-visit record. This permutation holds constant the total number of male and of female visits, and the total number of visits to each flower species from both sexes combined (Figure B in S1 File). The range of dissimilarity values from this simulation is the difference we would observe in our sample, if there were no true difference in flower species use between males and females of the same bee species. We evaluated the hypothesis that male and female diets overlap less than would be expected by chance; thus we use a one-sided alpha of p<0.05. We iterated this null simulation 9999 times, which was sufficient to stabilize p-values near our chosen alpha (North, Curtis & Sham 2002). When the observed dissimilarity was greater than 9500 of the 9999 simulated dissimilarities, we concluded that we had detected a difference in the pattern of floral visitation between conspecific male and female bees, given the observed diet breadth and abundance of each sex. We also computed the mean null model value, and a 95% confidence interval for this mean using the 0.025 and 0.975 quantiles of the dissimilarity values generated for each null model.

To compare the diet overlap we observed between sexes to a meaningful benchmark, interspecific diet overlap, we repeated the same null model analysis, this time comparing females of the focal species to females of other species. To compare the results, we present the difference between the observed dissimilarities and the null dissimilarities for each female-male and species-species comparison. We performed one analysis for each bee species for which we collected at least 20 visitation records for each sex (19 species). This sample size threshold is arbitrary, but null model variance shrinks with sample size, such that apparent patterns for species with smaller sample sizes are rarely interpretable (Figure C in S1 File).

Because we analyze 19 bee species, females of each species are compared to 18 others. We then compared the female-male difference (observed minus mean null dissimilarity in flower communities visited) to the analogous species-species difference (observed minus null dissimilarity).

For this analysis, which evaluates holistic differences between male and female bees of the same species, we combined observations across the full season and all sites. This allows us to observe foraging niche differences that are driven by flower and/ or bee phenology, in addition to any sex-specific floral preference.

2) To what degree are particular flower species disproportionately visited by bees of one sex?

To answer this question, we fit a random intercepts model to our entire data set of 153 bee species to determine whether particular flower species are disproportionately visited by male or female bees, and whether the answer varies by bee species. In our model, bee sex is the response, and flower species, bee species, site, and their interactions are all random effects; we included no fixed effects. The random effects provide partial pooling, which is especially useful when there are many levels, few data associated with some or all levels, and/ or inconsistent amounts of data across levels [34]. We can infer disproportionate visitation by male vs. female bees for a flower species when predicted odds of visitors to that flower species being male are especially high or low.

We statistically control for variation in the overall sex ratio across bee species through a random intercept of bee species, and variation in sex ratios across sites, through random intercepts for site, and the site-bee species and site-flower species interactions. Although we deemed it unlikely that, within bee species, sex ratios at birth vary greatly across space, any variability attributed to site terms could result from differences in bee sex ratios, or from differential overlap of bee foraging activity and flower bloom across space.

We call this model the “summed model” because we sum interactions observed across the entire season (all five sampling rounds) at each site. In the summed model, the relationship between phenological overlap and the odds of flower-visiting bees being male would be incorporated into the species effects. This perspective is helpful for considering flower species’ contributions to the overall diets of male versus female bees. We fit the model with the R package lme4 [35] with the following call:

Summed model

lme4::glmer(bee_sex ~ (1|site)+ (1|flower_species)+ (1|bee_species)+ (1|flower_species:bee_species)+ (1|site:bee_species)+ (1|site:flower_species), family = “binomial”, data = data)

We included bee species and site as random, rather than fixed, effects to directly compare the variability in bee sex associated with each of these predictors to the variability associated with flower species (preference). Comparing the overall variability across these groups was more important to us than assessing predictions on a per-site or per-bee-species basis. We fit flower species, the primary covariate of interest, as a random effect to facilitate model fitting (fewer degrees of freedom) as well as interpretation. In our summed model, we included all two-way interactions, but omitted the three-way interaction, bee species by flower species by site. Although the sort of context-dependent preference this term could represent (e.g. males from bee species 1 prefer flower A at one site (relative to females), but shun it at another) may exist in nature, it is unlikely we would estimate it accurately in our model.

We confirmed model convergence by comparing several fitting methods using the allFit function in lme4 [35], which all showed similar parameter estimates (Table A in S1 File). We tested whether residuals from our model were overdispersed using Bolker’s function “overdisp” [36], and visually assessed our additivity assumptions with binned residual plots [34] (Figure D in S1 File).

3) To what extent are differences in floral use driven by preference, rather than phenological differences between male and female bees?

Over the 11 weeks of our study, we observed turnover in bee species, in flower bloom, and within-bee species changes in sex ratio. Therefore, phenological overlap between male versus female bees and the bloom period of particular flower species, rather than preference of those bees for those flowers, may explain much of the variation in sex ratio we observed across visitors. In question 3, we are explicitly interested in distinguishing sex-specific diet preferences from variable use resulting from seasonal resource availability and male vs. female abundance. We do this in the “seasonal model” by incorporating sampling round (our measure of phenology) as an additional random intercept effect, along with random intercepts for the interactions between sampling round and the other covariates. We chose to include sampling round as a random effect because this enables direct comparison to all other terms in both models. We ignored the three- and four-way interactions between bee species, flower species, and other covariates. We fit this model with the following call in the R package lme4, with new terms in bold:

Seasonal model

glmer(bee_sex ~ (1|site)+ (1|flower_species)+ (1|bee_species)+ (1|flower_species:bee_species)+ (1|site:bee_species)+ (1|site:flower_species)+ (1|sampling_round)+ (1|site:sampling_round)+ (1|flower_species:sampling_round)+

(1| bee_species:sampling_round)+ (1|site:bee_species:sampling_round)+ (1|site:flower_species:sampling_round), family = “binomial”, data = data)

Our index of preference for both the summed model and the seasonal model is the change in predicted odds that a bee is male when the flower species it visits is given. To describe the importance of model terms, we calculated a bootstrapped median odds ratio using code from Seth [37], which gives the expected difference in odds that a flower-visiting bee is male between levels of a predictor [38]. For example, a median odds ratio of five for the main effect of sampling round would indicate that the odds of a flower-visiting bee being male differ by about a factor of five between sampling rounds, while a median odds ratio of one would indicate that the odds of a flower-visiting bee being male do not change across rounds. If the median odds ratio is large for flower species in both models (and the random effects predictions for each species are consistent across both models), we could say that there are intrinsic (i.e. not simply phenological) properties of flower species identity that male or female bees prefer. If flower species is a strong predictor of bee sex in the summed model but not in the seasonal one, we would still conclude that flower species often contribute more strongly to the diet of one sex than the other, though these differences may not arise due to differing preferences. If the sampling round terms have large median odds ratios, then accounting for phenology is critical for identifying differences in preference in addition to differences in use.

Because male bees require less pollen for their own diets and do not collect pollen to provision offspring, we predict that they would be less likely to visit flower species that do not produce nectar than females would. Post-hoc, we examined whether the predicted odds that visitors to nectarless flower species would be male were lower than for flower species known to produce both pollen and nectar in the seasonal model (Appendix A in S1 File).

Results

In total we collected 18,698 bee specimens belonging to 152 bee species (Table B in S1 File) from a total of 109 flower species (Table C in S1 File), which together comprised 1417 unique species-species interactions. Although the ratio of male to female bees was highly variable across bee species (Table B in S1 File), roughly 18% of specimens were male (n = 3372). Thus, the overall ratio of male to female bees we collected was 0.22, although this ratio varied markedly between flower species (Fig 1).

Male and female bees show large differences in floral preference (2)

The sex ratio (M:F) of flower-visiting bees varies across flower species.

Each point represents a flower species; the x-axis is the number of bees collected from that species, the y-axis is the ratio of male to female bees collected from the flower. Flower species that received >19 visits are plotted (n = 54). The shaded region is bounded by a smoothed fit to the 97.5th and 2.5th percentiles of the binomial distribution given by the observed ratio of males to females in our overall dataset (M/F = 0.22; i.e. M/(M+F) = 0.18). This distribution represents our expectation for random variation in sex ratio across flower species, if the sex ratio of flower-visiting bees is independent of flower species identity (male and female bees exhibit the same floral preferences), and remains nearly constant across time and space.

How much do male and female bee diets overlap?

We found that male and female bee diets overlap significantly less than would be expected given random sampling of the flowers visited by both sexes (Fig 2), and that the differences in diet composition between male and female bees of several species were of similar magnitude to the differences in diet between species of bee (Fig 3). The patterns we observed did not result from a single tendency across all bee species, such as males always visiting a nested subset of flower species visited by females (Figure E in S1 File).

Male and female bees show large differences in floral preference (3)

Flower visit patterns of male and female bees of the same species differed significantly.

Red points are observed Morisita-Horn dissimilarities between flower communities visited by all male and all female bees of a particular species across all sites and sampling rounds. Black points are the mean dissimilarity (gray bars, 95% CI) from a permutation-based null model that randomly shuffles the sex associated with each visit record, maintaining the total number of males, females, and overall combined visits to each floral species.

Male and female bees show large differences in floral preference (4)

The diets of male and female bees of the same species can be as dissimilar as the diets of females of two different bee species.

Dissimilarities in this figure are the observed statistic minus, for each pairwise comparison, the mean dissimilarity in the null model. Each panel focuses on a bee species (panel name) and shows: above the label “sex”, observed diet dissimilarity between male and female bees of the focal species, minus the average null dissimilarity resulting from randomly permuting the sex identity of each visit record; above the label “sp”, observed diet dissimilarity between female bees of the focal species and each other bee species, minus the average null dissimilarity resulting from randomly permuting the species identity of each visit record.

To what degree are particular flower species disproportionately visited by bees of one sex?

The sex ratio of flower-visiting bees varied across species of flower (Fig 2). After controlling for bee species identity (the strongest predictor of sex in our models, Fig 4), and site, we still found that some flower species received a disproportionate number of male bee visitors (Figs (Figs44 and and5).5). The median odds ratio for the main effect of flower species was 3.6 (bootstrapped CI 3.0–4.2) in our summed model, indicating that, typically, the visitor sex ratio differs between two flower species by more than a factor of 3. Furthermore, we observed sex-based differences in flower use specific to particular bee species: the median odds ratio for the flower species by bee species interaction in our summed model was nearly as large (median = 3.1, bootstrapped CI 3.0–3.3) as the main effect of flower species. By contrast, sex ratios did not differ between sites (median odds ratio for main effect of site = 1).

Male and female bees show large differences in floral preference (5)

Flower species, along with bee species, predicts the sex of visiting bees, indicating that floral preferences differ between male and female bees.

Flower species is an important predictor of bee sex even after accounting for phenology (seasonal model). For each term (“bee” = bee species, “flower” = flower species, “round” = sampling round) in each model, the median odds ratio (+/- 95% bootstrapped credible interval) indicates the expected difference in odds that a flower-visiting bee is male between two levels. For example, a median odds ratio of 3.7 for the flower species term means the odds of a visitor being male are expected to differ by a factor of 3.7 between two randomly selected species of flower.

Male and female bees show large differences in floral preference (6)

Male bee preferences for and against flower species vary across flower species.

Each point is the conditional mode of the random effects prediction (the random-effects analog to an estimate), for a flower species that received at least 20 visits, on the logit scale. Zero represents the odds of a visitor being male on a random flower, and -2 or 2 indicates a ~7 fold decrease or increase in those odds, given flower species identity. Error bars are the square root of the conditional variances on the conditional mode * 1.96, and can be interpreted as the expected range in which the random effect for a particular flower truly lies, analogous to 1.96 times the standard error of the mean for a fixed effect.

To what extent are differences in floral use driven by preference, rather than phenological differences between male and female bees?

The flower species blooming in our system turned over throughout our 11-week sampling period, with several highly visited species blooming for only one of the three months during which we sampled. This turnover, along with potential sex-specific bee flight seasons (e.g. males emerge first in many solitary species, but are not produced until the end of the colony cycle for many social species), means that differences in diet between male and female bees could reflect seasonal availability and use, without also indicating preference differences between the sexes. Indeed, phenology predicts bee sex somewhat, with the odds of a flower-visiting bee being male expected to change by a factor of 1.5 (bootstrapped CI 1.1–1.9) between sampling rounds (Fig 4). Phenological patterns of male vs. female flight seasons vary across bee species; the median odds ratio for the bee species by sampling round interaction is 2.2 (bootstrapped CI 2.1–2.3) (Fig 4). Even after accounting for these effects, however, there remains a strong association between the species of flower a bee visits and its sex (Figs (Figs44 and and55).

The relative effects of each flower species on the sex of its visitors were changed very little by accounting for phenology; Pearson and Spearman correlations between the random effect of flower species in the seasonal model and the same random effect in the simpler summed model were both 0.98. Each flower species recorded in our study appears in Table C in S1 File, along with the number of male and female bees collected from it and the conditional mode of the random effect prediction from the seasonal model. In addition to finding overall preference difference between males and females, we found evidence for bee-species-specific difference in floral preferences between the sexes (median odds ratios in both models for the bee species by flower species interaction > 2.8). A post-hoc examination of random effects predictions was consistent with our prediction that male bees avoid flower species that do not produce nectar, with the odds of visiting bees being male nearly twice as high, on average, for flower species that do produce nectar (Figure E in S1 File).

Discussion

We found strong evidence for sexually dimorphic foraging preferences in bees. At the level of individual flower species, we found that commonly, a disproportionate number of bee visitors were male, and that sexually dimorphic preferences drove these patterns. We found that the difference between the flower species visited by male and female bees of the same species was similar in magnitude to differences between females of different species. The partitioning of the floral community among bee species is a primary focus of pollination ecology and ecological network analysis [39], but male bees are typically disregarded or lumped together with their female counterparts. Our study suggests this may represent an important oversight. Further, our study provides strong confirmation of the few studies that investigate the foraging behavior of male bees, which found that males play a unique role in plant pollination [2123,40]. Lastly, our result implies that male bees contribute substantially to the complexity of plant-pollinator networks in nature, and that network analyses might benefit from separating males and females into different nodes [2,3].

Phenology, a previously reported mechanism for distinct use of floral resources by male and female bees [21,41], explained some variation in the sex ratio of flower-visiting bees, but was less important than flower species identity over the period of our study. We expected to find an effect of phenology because both the identity of the flower species blooming within sites, and also the sex ratio of foragers within bee species, vary across the season. Males emerge first in most solitary bees. In contrast, for social species, initial broods usually consist primarily of female workers, then males and reproductive females emerge at the end of the colony cycle [15]. To account for the possibility that phenology explained the disproportionate use of many flower species by one sex of bee, we extended our summed model by adding phenology terms. Surprisingly, phenology only weakly predicted the sex of flower-visiting bees. This is despite the fact that, as predicted by natural history, the sampling round(s) in which males were relatively more prevalent depended on bee species (the bee species by sampling round interaction was much bigger than the sampling round main effect; Fig 4). This indicates that our evidence for floral preference differences between male and female bees was robust to accounting for seasonal turnover in flower species bloom, bee species flight seasons, and the sex ratios within bee species.

Patterns in bee-flower interaction data can arise from the sampling process itself [42,43]. Our analyses control for these patterns. To evaluate diet overlap, we used a dissimilarity index that downweights rarely used resources, and implemented a null model that accounts for differences that could arise from sampling effects or the fact that females outnumber males in our dataset by nearly a factor of 5. To evaluate preference, we used random effects models that incorporated all (nearly 19,000) observations, and shrank extreme values for rarely observed species-species interactions towards the global mean for each effect. Thus, our estimates for sex-specific preferences should be robust to the inevitable under-sampling of rarer taxa. Establishing differences in preference between categories of bees such as males and females, even when resource availability is seasonal and difficult to quantify, is possible using methods such as these, though absolute preference remains elusive.

Some studies show, and conservation practice assumes that floral diversity is associated with more bee individuals and diversity, although this pattern could arise from many processes [4448], Complementary flower species use between the sexes implies one mechanism by which a bee species could benefit from a diversity of flower choices: a resource used in small proportions at the species level may be crucial for fitness in one sex. Such a dependency would likely be overlooked when individuals of both sexes are pooled before analysis.

Within pollinating insects, sexually dimorphic preferences and contribution to plant reproduction have been reported before [4951]. Though studies examining foraging differences between male and female bees [5255] and pollination by male bees [22,23,40,5658] have been few outside sexual mimicry and scent collection pollination systems, they found that male and female bees visit different flowers, and that male bees could be important pollinators. Male bees may be especially implicated in long-distance pollen transfer [18,5860], although outside the tropics there is little direct evidence this is true.

Mating behaviors of male bees [11,15,6166] are better known than foraging behaviors. The two, however, are likely closely linked. Mating-related selection may drive differences in the sensory systems of male and female bees [6769], or even their approaches to learning [70,71]. The mate seeking behaviors of male bees, such as patrolling routes [65] or seeking flowers visited by conspecific females [72] could generate differences from females via complementarity (males visiting flower species not visited by females), or nestedness (one sex primarily visiting a subset of species visited by the other). We found evidence for both (Figure E in S1 File). Divergent floral preferences between sexes may reflect nutritional needs or mating behavior, but could also reflect visual or olfactory sensitivities that differ between the sexes [67,68].

Differences in body size and thermal ecology between male and female bees may also determine foraging behavior [15]. In animals, size dimorphisms often mediate trophic relationships [4,6,8,7375]. Body size often manifests ecologically through thermal constraints, which also drive bee-plant interactions [7679]. In fact, male bees may have preferences relative to females for some flower species based solely on their thermal rewards [80].

Lastly, nutritional rewards likely drive differences in flower species preference between male and female bees. Whereas most female bees collect both nectar and pollen, male bees forage primarily for nectar to fuel flight [15]. Thus, we predicted that male bees would avoid flowers that produce no nectar. Indeed, in both our models, the predicted odds of a bee visiting a nectar-less flower species being male were approximately half that of a bee visiting flower species that produce nectar (Figure E in S1 File). Within flower species that produce pollen and nectar, we found large variation in the relative preferences of male and female bees. Further investigation could reveal which floral traits mediate these sex-specific flower preferences and visitation rates in bees.

Scaling up, it is currently unknown how the distinct foraging niches of male bees mediate either the robustness of pollinator communities to species loss and environmental perturbations [14,81,82], or the effectiveness of different habitat ameliorations [49,55,83]. This study suggests that both questions warrant further investigation.

Supporting information

S1 File

Sampling scheme.

(a) The six study sites in central New Jersey, USA. (b) Schematic sampling diagram (not to scale). One observer walked parallel 2m transects covering the entire sampling area. Each 30-minute sampling bout resumed where the previous one left off; observers typically covered the entire meadow once over a 3-day sampling round. (c) The southwestern-most site in peak bloom (Figure A). Schematic cartoon of our simulation for the dissimilarity values associated with our null hypothesis that diets of male and female bees do not differ. (a) Each collection record for each bee species associates the sex of an individual bee to the flower species from which it was collected. (b) To compute the dissimilarity between males and females, we compare all visits to each flower species from males (purple vector) to all visits to each flower species from females (green vector). (c) The Morisita-Horn index summarizes the differences between the two vectors as a value between 0 (identical) and 1 (maximally dissimilar). (d) For our null model, we shuffle the sex column from our observation table. (e) This produces two null vectors. The row and column sums for the matrices in (b) and (c) are identical, but the elements can differ. (f) For our null model, we compute the dissimilarity between the null vectors. We repeated steps d-f 9999 times to generate confidence intervals for the null hypothesis that the sex of a visiting bee is unrelated to the flower species it is collected from. When comparing the flower species visited by different species of bee, we conducted an analysis identical except that rather than comparing two sexes of the same species, we compared two species of the same sex (i.e. exchanging “sex” and “species” throughout Figure A in S1 File) (Figure B). Effect size for diet dissimilarity is independent of sample size, while standardized effect is strongly driven by the number of individuals of the sex with the fewest records. a) Observed Morisita-Horn dissimilarity in flower communities visited by male and female bees of a single species, minus average null dissimilarity vs. the number of records for the less frequently observed sex. b) Observed minus null dissimilarity in composition of flowers visited by male and female bees of a single species, scaled by the variation in the null model, versus the number of records for the less frequently observed sex (Figure C). Binned residual plots for each model show minor violation of the additivity assumption. Residuals and predicted values on the probability scale (Figure D). Seasonal model predictions are consistent with the hypothesis that male bees avoid flower species that do not produce nectar, relative to females. Each point is the random effect prediction (change in odds that a bee visiting that flower is male) for a flower species. Boxplots show the 25th, 50th, and 75th percentiles, with whiskers extending to more extreme values within 1.5x the interquartile range (Figure E). Methods for post-hoc analysis of male avoidance of nectar-free flowers (Appendix A). Model convergence confirmed based on similar parameter estimates across fitting routines. For each model, the estimate for each term is given for each of 6 fitting algorithms in the R package lme4. Subsequent analyses used parameter estimates in yellow, in both cases tied for the highest estimated likelihood with other very similar fits (Table A). Bee species with number of female and male specimens collected (Table B). Number of male and female visitors to each plant species, and bias towards attracting male bee visitors. This bias is the random effect prediction from the seasonal model, which indicates the change in log(odds) that a visiting bee is male when the species of flower it visits is given; greater values indicate male bias (Table C).

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

We thank Daniel Cariveau and Neal Williams for study design suggestions, and Jason Gibbs, Tina Harrison, Dylan Simpson, and three anonymous reviewers for comments on an earlier draft. Cameron Kanterman and Riva Letchinger helped collect field data; Kurtis Himmler, Tiffany Bennet, and professional taxonomists Jason Gibbs, Joel Gardiner, and Sam Droege helped with bee species determinations. Mercer County, the Institute for Advanced Study, Somerset County, and the Raritan Headwaters Association provided site permission.

Funding Statement

Funded by MR NSFDGE1433187 National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship. https://www.nsfgrfp.org// RW USDA-NRCS Conservation Innovation Grant Program 2012. United States Department of Agriculture Natural Resource conservation Services. https://www.nrcs.usda.gov/wps/portal/nrcs/main/national/programs/financial/cig/. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Data Availability

All bee specimens are curated in the Winfree lab collection at Rutgers University, and the data used in this paper, along with R scripts used in data analysis and figure preparation, are available from the Dryad Digital Repository (doi:10.5061/dryad.c3rr6q1).

References

1. Durell SEALV dit. Individual feeding specialization in shorebirds: population consequences and conservation implications. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc. Rutgers University; 2000;75: 503–518. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

2. Bolnick DI, Amarasekare P, Araújo MS, Bürger R, Levine JM, Novak M, et al. Why intraspecific trait variation matters in community ecology. Trends Ecol Evol. 2011;26: 183–192. 10.1016/j.tree.2011.01.009 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

3. Zwolak R.How intraspecific variation in seed-dispersing animals matters for plants. Biol Rev. 2018;93: 897–913. 10.1111/brv.12377 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

4. Temeles EJ, Miller JS, Rifkin JL. Evolution of sexual dimorphism in bill size and shape of hermit hummingbirds (Phaethornithinae): a role for ecological causation. Philos Trans R Soc B. 2010;365: 1053–1063. 10.1098/rstb.2009.0284 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

5. Butler MA, Sawyer SA, Losos JB. Sexual dimorphism and adaptive radiation in Anolis lizards. Nature. 2007;447: 202–5. 10.1038/nature05774 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

6. Start D, De Lisle S. Sexual dimorphism in a top predator (Notophthalmus viridescens) drives aquatic prey community assembly. Proc R Soc B. 2018;285 10.1098/rspb.2018.1717 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

7. Fryxell DC, Arnett HA, Apgar TM, Kinnison MT, Palkovacs EP. Sex ratio variation shapes the ecological effects of a globally introduced freshwater fish. Proc R Soc B. 2015;282: 20151970 10.1098/rspb.2015.1970 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

8. Selander RK. Sexual dimorphism and differential iche utilization in birds. Condor. 1966;68: 113–151. 10.2307/1365712 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

9. Shine R.Ecological causes for the evolution of sexual dimorphism: a review of the evidence. Q Rev Biol. 1989;64: 419–461. Available: http://www.jstor.org/stable/2830103 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

10. Willmer P. Pollination and floral ecology. Princeton: Princeton University Press; 2011. [Google Scholar]

11. Alco*ck J, Barrows EM, Gordh G, Hubbard LJ, Kirkendall L, Pyle DW, et al. The ecology and evolution of male reproductive behaviour in the bees and wasps. Zool J Linn Soc. 1978;64: 293–326. 10.1111/j.1096-3642.1978.tb01075.x [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

12. Heinrich B.“Majoring” and “Minoring” by foraging bumblebees, Bombus vagans: an experimental analysis. Ecology. 1979;60: 245–255. [Google Scholar]

13. Bruninga-Socolar B, Crone EE, Winfree R. The role of floral density in determining bee foraging behavior: a natural experiment. Nat Areas J. 2016;36: 392–399. 10.3375/043.036.0406 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

14. Tur C, Vigalondo B, Trøjelsgaard K, Olesen JM, Traveset A. Downscaling pollen-transport networks to the level of individuals. J Anim Ecol. 2014;83: 306–317. 10.1111/1365-2656.12130 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

15. Willmer PG, Stone GN. Behavioral, ecological, and physiological determinants of the activity patterns of bees. Adv Study Behav. 2004;34: 347–466. [Google Scholar]

16. Cane JH, Sipes SS. Characterizing floral specialization by bees: Analytical methods and a revised lexicon for oligolecty. Plant–pollinator interactions: From specialization to generalization. 2006. 10.1007/BF00937854 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

17. Parachnowitsch AL, Manson JS, Sletvold N. Evolutionary ecology of nectar. Ann Bot. 2018; 247–261. 10.1093/aob/mcy132 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

18. Janzen DH. Euglossine bees as long-distance pollinators of tropical plants. Science (80-). 1971;171: 203–205. 10.1126/science.171.3967.203 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

19. Eltz T, Zimmermann Y, Haftmann J, Twele R, Francke W, Quezada-Euan JJG, et al. Enfleurage, lipid recycling and the origin of perfume collection in orchid bees. Proc R Soc—B. 2007;274: 2843–2848. 10.1098/rspb.2007.0727 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

20. Etl F, Franschitz A, Aguiar AJC, Schönenberger J, Dötterl S. A perfume-collecting male oil bee? Evidences of a novel pollination system involving Anthurium acutifolium (Araceae) and Paratetrapedia chocoensis (Apidae, Tapinotaspidini). Flora Morphol Distrib Funct Ecol Plants. Elsevier GmbH.; 2017;232: 7–15. 10.1016/j.flora.2017.02.020 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

21. Ogilvie JE, Thomson JD. Male bumble bees are important pollinators of a late-blooming plant. Arthropod Plant Interact. 2015;9: 205–213. 10.1007/s11829-015-9368-x [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

22. Cane JH, Sampson BJ, Miller SA. Pollination value of male bees: The specialist bee Peponapis pruinosa (Apidae) at Summer Squash (Cucurbita pepo). Environ Entomol. 2011;40: 614–620. 10.1603/EN10084 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

23. Cane JH. Pollinating bees (Hymenoptera: Apiformes) of U.S. alfalfa compared for rates of pod and seed set. J Econ Entomol. 2002;95: 22–27. 10.1603/0022-0493-95.1.22 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

24. Straub L, Villamar-Bouza L, Bruckner S, Chantawannakul P, Gauthier L, Khongphinitbunjong K, et al. Neonicotinoid insecticides can serve as inadvertent insect contraceptives. Proc R Soc—B. 2016;283 10.1098/rspb.2016.0506 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

25. Elias J, Dorn S, Mazzi D. No evidence for increased extinction proneness with decreasing effective population size in a parasitoid with complementary sex determination and fertile diploid males. BMC Evol Biol. 2010;10: 366 10.1186/1471-2148-10-366 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

26. Gloag RS, Christie JR, Ding G, Stephens RE, Buchmann G, Oldroyd BP. Workers’ sons rescue genetic diversity at the sex locus in an invasive honey bee population. Mol Ecol. 2019; 0–2. 10.1111/mec.15031 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

27. López-Uribe MM, Morreale SJ, Santiago CK, Danforth BN. Nest suitability, fine-scale population structure and male-mediated dispersal of a solitary ground nesting bee in an urban landscape. PLoS One. 2015;10: 1–20. 10.1371/journal.pone.0125719 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

28. Ulrich Y, Perrin N, Chapuisat M. Flexible social organization and high incidence of drifting in the sweat bee, Halictus scabiosae. Mol Ecol. 2009;18: 1791–1800. 10.1111/j.1365-294X.2009.04154.x [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

29. Hicks DM, Ouvrard P, Baldock KCR, Baude M, Goddard MA, Kunin WE, et al. Food for pollinators: quantifying the nectar and pollen resources of urban flower meadows. PLoS One. 2016;11: 1–37. 10.1371/journal.pone.0158117 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

30. R Core Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing [Internet]. Vienna, Austria; 2018. Available: https://www.r-project.org/ [Google Scholar]

31. Morisita M.Measuring of interspecific association and similarity between communities [Internet]. Memoirs of the Faculty of Science, Kyushu University, Series E (Biology). 1959. pp. 65–80. Available: http://reference.morisita.gr.jp/paper_pdf/56.pdf [Google Scholar]

32. Horn HS. Measurement of “Overlap” in comparative ecological studies. Am Nat. 1966;100: 419–424. Available: http://www.jstor.org/stable/2459242 [Google Scholar]

33. Barwell LJ, Isaac NJB, Kunin WE. Measuring beta-diversity with species abundance data. J Anim Ecol. 2015; 1112–1122. 10.1111/1365-2656.12362 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

34. Gelman A, Hill J. Data analysis using regression and multilevel/ hierarchical models. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2007. [Google Scholar]

35. Bates D, Maechler M, Walker S. Package “lme4” [Internet]. CRAN Repository. 2016. p. 113 10.18637/jss.v067.i01 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

36. Bolker BM. GLMM FAQ [Internet]. 2017. Available: http://bbolker.github.io/mixedmodels-misc/glmmFAQ.html

37. Seth M. How to implement credible 95% interval for median odds ratio using JAGS? [Internet]. 2017 [cited 9 May 2018]. Available: https://stats.stackexchange.com/q/318912

38. Merlo J, Chaix B, Ohlsson H, Beckman A, Johnell K, Hjerpe P, et al. A brief conceptual tutorial of multilevel analysis in social epidemiology: Using measures of clustering in multilevel logistic regression to investigate contextual phenomena. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2006;60: 290–297. 10.1136/jech.2004.029454 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

39. Bascompte J, Jordano P. Mutualistic networks in time and space In: Bascompte J, Jordano P, editors. Mutualistic networks. Princeton University Press; 2014. pp. 87–106. Available: http://www.jstor.org/stable/j.ctt5hhnpq.8 [Google Scholar]

40. Pascarella JB. Pollination biology of Gelsemium sempervirens L. (Ait.) (Gelsemiaceae): do male and female Habropoda laboriosa F. (Hymenoptera, Apidae) differ in pollination efficiency?J Apic Res. 2010;49: 170–176. 10.3896/IBRA.1.49.2.05 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

41. Robertson C.Heterotropic bees. Ecology. 1925;6: 412–436. Available: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1941781 [Google Scholar]

42. Blüthgen N.Why network analysis is often disconnected from community ecology: A critique and an ecologist’s guide. Basic Appl Ecol. 2010;11: 185–195. 10.1016/j.baae.2010.01.001 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

43. Fründ J, McCann KS, Williams NM. Sampling bias is a challenge for quantifying specialization and network structure: Lessons from a quantitative niche model. Oikos. 2016;125: 502–513. 10.1111/oik.02256 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

44. Senapathi D, Goddard MA, Kunin WE, Baldock KCR. Landscape impacts on pollinator communities in temperate systems: evidence and knowledge gaps. Funct Ecol. 2016; 10.1111/1365-2435.12809 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

45. De Palma A, Kuhlmann M, Roberts SPM, Potts SG, Börger L, Hudson LN, et al. Ecological traits affect the sensitivity of bees to land-use pressures in European agricultural landscapes. J Appl Ecol. 2015;52: 1567–1577. 10.1111/1365-2664.12524 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

46. Roulston TH, Goodell K. The role of resources and risks in regulating wild bee populations. Annu Rev Entomol. 2011;56: 293–312. 10.1146/annurev-ento-120709-144802 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

47. Spiesman BJ, Bennett A, Isaacs R, Gratton C. Bumble bee colony growth and reproduction depend on local flower dominance and natural habitat area in the surrounding landscape. Biol Conserv. Elsevier Ltd; 2017;206: 217–223. 10.1016/j.biocon.2016.12.008 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

48. Sutter L, Jeanneret P, Bartual AM, Bocci G, Albrecht M. Enhancing plant diversity in agricultural landscapes promotes both rare bees and dominant crop-pollinating bees through complementary increase in key floral resources. J Appl Ecol. 2017;54: 1856–1864. 10.1111/1365-2664.12907 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

49. Rusterholtz H-P, Erhardt A. Can nectar properties explain sex-specific flower preferences in the Adonis Blue butterfly Lysandra bellargus?Ecol Entomol. 2000;25: 81–90. [Google Scholar]

50. Broadhead GT, Basu T, von Arx M, Raguso RA. Diel rhythms and sex differences in the locomotor activity of hawkmoths. J Exp Biol. 2017;220: 1472–1480. 10.1242/jeb.143966 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

51. Alarcón R, Riffell JA, Davidowitz G, Hildebrand JG, Bronstein JL. Sex-dependent variation in the floral preferences of the hawkmoth Manduca sexta. Anim Behav. 2010;80: 289–296. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.05.007 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

52. Ne’eman G, Shavit O, Shaltiel L, Shmida A. Foraging by male and female solitary bees with implications for pollination. J Insect Behav. 2006;19: 383–401. 10.1007/s10905-006-9030-7 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

53. Ritchie AD, Ruppel R, Jha S. Generalist behavior describes pollen foraging for perceived oligolectic and polylectic bees. Environ Entomol. 2016;45: 909–919. 10.1093/ee/nvw032 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

54. Ogilvie JE, Forrest JR. Interactions between bee foraging and floral resource phenology shape bee populations and communities. Curr Opin Insect Sci. Elsevier Inc; 2017;21: 75–82. 10.1016/j.cois.2017.05.015 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

55. Rundlöf M, Persson AS, Smith HG, Bommarco R. Late-season mass-flowering red clover increases bumble bee queen and male densities. Biol Conserv. 2014;172: 138–145. 10.1016/j.biocon.2014.02.027 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

56. Fliszkiewicz M, Giejdasz K, Wilkaniec Z. The importance of male red mason bee (Osmia rufa L.) and male bufftailed bumblebee (Bombus terrestris L.) pollination in blackcurrant (Ribes nigrum L.). J Hortic Sci Biotechnol. 2011;86: 457–460. 10.1080/14620316.2011.11512788 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

57. Ostevik K, Manson J, Thomson J. Pollination potential of male bumble bees (Bombus impatiens): movement patterns and pollen-transfer efficiency. J Pollinat Ecol. 2010;2: 21–26. Available: http://www.pollinationecology.org/index.php?journal=jpe&page=article&op=view&path[]=100 [Google Scholar]

58. Wolf S, Moritz RFA. The pollination potential of free-foraging bumblebee (Bombus spp.) males (Hymenoptera: Apidae). Apidologie. 2014;45: 440–450. 10.1007/s13592-013-0259-9 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

59. Kraus FB, Wolf S, Moritz RFA. Male flight distance and population substructure in the bumblebee Bombus terrestris. J Anim Ecol. 2009;78: 247–252. 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2008.01479.x [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

60. Roubik DW. Tropical pollinators in the canopy and understory: Field data and theory for stratum “preferences.”J Insect Behav. 1993;6: 659–673. 10.1007/BF01201668 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

61. Alco*ck J.Male behvaiour in two bumblebees, Bombus nevadensis auricomus and B. griseicollis (Hymenoptera: Apidae). J Zool. 1983;200: 561–570. [Google Scholar]

62. Alco*ck J.Sexual selection and the mating behavior of solitary bees [Internet]. Advances in the Study of Behavior. Elsevier Inc.; 2013. 10.1016/B978-0-12-407186-5.00001-X [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

63. Stone GN. Female foraging responses to sexual harassment in the solitary bee Anthophora plumipes. Anim Behav. 1995;50: 405–412. 10.1006/anbe.1995.0255 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

64. Pinheiro M, Alves-dos-Santos I, Sazima M. Flowers as sleeping places for male bees: somehow the males know which flowers their females prefer. Arthropod Plant Interact. Springer Netherlands; 2017;11: 329–337. 10.1007/s11829-017-9532-6 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

65. Barrows EM. Mating behavior in halictine bees (Hymenoptera: Halictidae): II. microterritorial and patrolling behavior in males of Lasioglossum rohweri. Zeitschrift Fur Tierpsychologie-Journal Comp Ethol. 1976;40: 377–389. [Google Scholar]

66. Barrows EM. Mating behavior in halictine bees (Hymenoptera: Halictidae): I. patrolling and age-specific behavior in males. J Kansas Entomol Soc. 1976;49: 105–119. Available: http://www.jstor.org/stable/25082792 [Google Scholar]

67. Streinzer M, Kelber C, Pfabigan S, Kleineidam CJ, Spaethe J. Sexual dimorphism in the olfactory system of a solitary and a eusocial bee species. J Comp Neurol. 2013;521: 2742–2755. 10.1002/cne.23312 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

68. Somanathan H, Borges RM, Warrant EJ, Kelber A. Visual adaptations for mate detection in the male carpenter bee Xylocopa tenuiscapa. PLoS One. 2017;12 10.1371/journal.pone.0168452 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

69. Brand P, Larcher V, Couto A, Sandoz JC, Ramírez SR. Sexual dimorphism in visual and olfactory brain centers in the perfume-collecting orchid bee Euglossa dilemma (Hymenoptera, Apidae). J Comp Neurol. 2018;526: 2068–2077. 10.1002/cne.24483 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

70. Robert T, Frasnelli E, Collett TS, de Ibarra NH. Male bumblebees perform learning flights on leaving a flower but not when leaving their nest. J Exp Biol. 2016;220: 930–937. 10.1242/jeb.151126 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

71. Dötterl S, Milchreit K, Schäffler I. Behavioural plasticity and sex differences in host finding of a specialized bee species. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sensory, Neural, Behav Physiol. 2011;197: 1119–1126. 10.1007/s00359-011-0673-2 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

72. Rossi BH, Nonacs P, Pitts-Singer TL. Sexual harassment by males reduces female fecundity in the alfalfa leafcutting bee, Megachile rotundata. Anim Behav. Elsevier Ltd; 2010;79: 165–171. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.10.023 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

73. Alonso JC, Salgado I, Palacín C. Thermal tolerance may cause sexual segregation in sexually dimorphic species living in hot environments. Behav Ecol. 2016;27: 717–724. 10.1093/beheco/arv211 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

74. Beck CA, Iverson SJ, Bowen WD, Blanchard W. Sex differences in grey seal diet reflect seasonal variation in foraging behaviour and reproductive expenditure: Evidence from quantitative fatty acid signature analysis. J Anim Ecol. 2007;76: 490–502. 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2007.01215.x [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

75. Givens RP. Dimorphic foraging strategies of a salticid spider (Phidippus audax). Ecology. 1978;59: 309–321. [Google Scholar]

76. Stone GN, Gilbert F, Willmer P, Potts S, Semida F, Zalat S. Windows of opportunity and the temporal structuring of foraging activity in a desert solitary bee. Ecol Entomol. 1999;24: 208–221. 10.1046/j.1365-2311.1999.00181.x [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

77. Chappell MA. Temperature regulation and energetics of the solitary bee Centris pallida during foraging and intermale mate competition. Physiol Zool. 1984;57: 215–225. 10.1086/physzool.57.2.30163707 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

78. Heinrich B, Heinrich MJE. Size and caste in temperature regulation by bumblebees. Physiol Zool. 1983;56: 552–562. [Google Scholar]

79. Rader R, Reilly J, Bartomeus I, Winfree R. Native bees buffer the negative impact of climate warming on honey bee pollination of watermelon crops. Glob Chang Biol. 2013;19: 3103–3110. 10.1111/gcb.12264 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

80. Sapir Y, Shmida A, Ne’eman G. Morning floral heat as a reward to the pollinators of the Oncocyclus irises. Oecologia. 2006;147: 53–59. 10.1007/s00442-005-0246-6 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

81. Ramos-Jiliberto R, Valdovinos FS, Moisset de Espanés P, Flores JD. Topological plasticity increases robustness of mutualistic networks. J Anim Ecol. 2012;81: 896–904. 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2012.01960.x [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

82. Brosi BJ, Briggs HM. Single pollinator species losses reduce floral fidelity and plant reproductive function. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2013;110: 13044–8. 10.1073/pnas.1307438110 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

83. Williams NM, Lonsdorf E V. Selecting cost-effective plant mixes to support pollinators. Biol Conserv. Elsevier; 2018;217: 195–202. 10.1016/j.biocon.2017.10.032 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

Male and female bees show large differences in floral preference (2024)

FAQs

What is the difference between female and male bees? ›

Different anatomy:

Firstly, male bees are larger than female worker bees. Yet they are smaller than the queen bee. The male bee has larger eyes and therefore better eyesight. This is important because the male bee needs to spot the queen bee.

Do bees have a preference for flowers? ›

We can use these traits to predict what pollinators might be attracted to certain flowers or we can use these tools to guide us to pick the right plant for the right pollinator! Bees, for example, are most attracted to flowers that have white, yellow, blue, or ultra-violet blooms.

Are bees on flowers male or female? ›

Female bees build, maintain, collect food for and defend nests, while male bees primarily seek mates. Both sexes drink floral nectar for food, but only females collect pollen that serves as food for young bees, so they forage at greater rates than males.

What is the symbiotic relationship between flowers and bees? ›

Bees and flowers have evolved together for millions of years. It is a mutual relationship where the bee is provided with food (nectar or pollen) and the stationary plant gets to disperse its pollen (sperm cells) to other plants of the same species.

How do bees choose gender? ›

A fertilized egg will become a female honeybee – either a worker bee or a queen bee. An unfertilized egg will develop into a male honeybee – destined to be a drone.

Why do female bees remove male bees? ›

You might think that most drones die because of sex, but this is not the case. In the fall, when foraging becomes scarce, drones become just another mouth to feed, but without contributing to the hive. Therefore, worker bees kick drones out of the hive, leading to their death.

Do bees prefer yellow or white flowers? ›

Bees are attracted to flowers based on the color, shape, and texture. They prefer flowers that are white, blue, yellow, and pink in color.

Do bees love all flowers? ›

A flower's color, odor, shape, size, timing, and reward (nectar or pollen) can increase or decrease the number of visits by specific pollinators. For example, bees can see UV light, but not red light, therefore they are attracted to flowers that are in the ultraviolet color range rather than red-hued.

Do male bees sleep in flowers? ›

Have you ever wondered where bees go to sleep at night? If you take a look in your garden early in the morning you will see some species sleeping right on the flowers! Male bees don't have a nest to go home to, so some find a cozy flower to rest on.

Are bees only female? ›

Of the 60,000 bees in a hive, almost 99% of them are female! Female honey bees, or worker bees, make all of the decisions in the hive and do all of the work. There are a couple hundred male bees in a hive, but they don't do much but sit around and eat food.

Do female bees need male bees? ›

Most animals reproduce sexually, which means that both males and females are required for the species to survive. Normally, the honeybee is no exception to this rule: the female queen bee produces new offspring by laying eggs that have been fertilised by sperm from male drones.

Do bees help flowers? ›

In return for a delicious meal, these animals help the plants reproduce by moving pollen between the male (anther) and female (stigma) parts of a flower. This act of pollination is the first step in generating seeds, which create new plants.

What would happen if bees went extinct? ›

Without bees, the availability and diversity of fresh produce would decline substantially, and human nutrition would likely suffer. Crops that would not be cost-effective to hand- or robot-pollinate would likely be lost or persist only with the dedication of human hobbyists.

How to tell a male bee from a female? ›

Looking at the length of the antennae can help you to distinguish between male and female bees. Males tend to have longer, more curved antennae while females have straighter and shorter antennae. The thorax is the middle segment of the bee, behind the head and before the abdomen.

Do male or female bees sting you? ›

A bee's stinger is a modified egg-laying structure, or ovipositor, so only females are capable of stinging. If you are REALLY confident differentiating between males and females, you could scoop up a male bee from a flower and not get stung!

Can female bees become male? ›

Unfertilized eggs become drone honey bees, while fertilized eggs develop into female workers and queens. Female workers do not mate, but they can lay infertile eggs, which in turn become male honey bees.

Are all the bees you see female? ›

Of the 60,000 bees in a hive, almost 99% of them are female! Female honey bees, or worker bees, make all of the decisions in the hive and do all of the work. There are a couple hundred male bees in a hive, but they don't do much but sit around and eat food. They don't even clean up after themselves!

Top Articles
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Lidia Grady

Last Updated:

Views: 5739

Rating: 4.4 / 5 (45 voted)

Reviews: 84% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Lidia Grady

Birthday: 1992-01-22

Address: Suite 493 356 Dale Fall, New Wanda, RI 52485

Phone: +29914464387516

Job: Customer Engineer

Hobby: Cryptography, Writing, Dowsing, Stand-up comedy, Calligraphy, Web surfing, Ghost hunting

Introduction: My name is Lidia Grady, I am a thankful, fine, glamorous, lucky, lively, pleasant, shiny person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.