Babies Smell Wonderful to Their Parents, Teenagers Do Not: an Exploratory Questionnaire Study on Children’s Age and Personal Odor Ratings in a Polish Sample (2024)

Babies Smell Wonderful to Their Parents, Teenagers Do Not: an Exploratory Questionnaire Study on Children’s Age and Personal Odor Ratings in a Polish Sample (1)

Link to Publisher's site

Chemosensory Perception

Chemosens Percept. 2017; 10(3): 81–87.

Published online 2017 Jun 27. doi:10.1007/s12078-017-9230-x

PMCID: PMC5574933

PMID: 28932343

Author information Article notes Copyright and License information Disclaimer

Abstract

Introduction

Infant body odor is subjectively pleasant to parents and activates reward areas in the brain. Hence, body odor perception might contribute to parental bonding. However, it is unknown whether the perceived pleasantness of children’s body odor varies over the course of a child’s development.

Methods

Two hundred and thirty-five parents (M=36.9years, SD=7.3) were asked to assess the personal odor pleasantness of their children (N=367; M=9.3years, SD=6.4).

Results

Odor pleasantness was found to decrease as a function of children’s age. Neither sex of the parent nor sex of the child contributed significantly to this effect.

Conclusions

We propose that the effect of age-related changes on personal odor pleasantness reflects olfactory modulation of parental-child relationships.

Implications

Our study suggests that perception of young children’s personal odor as pleasant may contribute to bonding and thereby caretaking, which is needed to a lesser degree after puberty than before.

Keywords: Body odor, Personal odor, Olfaction, Bonding, Child development

Introduction

Parents typically report that the odor of their baby is one of the most pleasant scents that they can imagine. Women who were born without the sense of smell declare that they regret missing out on this seemingly wonderful experience (Bojanowski et al. 2013). Within days (Schaal et al. 1980), or even hours of giving birth, mothers can distinguish the smell of their child from the smell of other babies (Kaitz et al. 1987; Porter et al. 1983). Body odors of pre-pubertal children are evaluated as very pleasant in general (Ferdenzi et al. 2010; Fleming et al. 1993), and the sheer presence of a baby body odor activates reward-related brain areas (Lundstrom et al. 2013).

It seems that the perception of body odors may contribute to parent-child bonding. For example, the ability to recognize one’s own child fosters the expression of warm feelings toward the baby (Corter and Fleming 1995; Del Cerro 1998; Depue and Morrone-Strupinsky 2005). Affective bonds with the caregiver are essential for a child’s social and emotional development, and they are strongly dependent on initial caretaking (Bowlby 1988). Caretaking does, however, bear costs on the parents—in modern, Western societies, it is time-consuming and expensive. In addition, parents often prioritize the children’s needs above their own and the safety of the child becomes their major concern (Leckman et al. 1999). In the long term, caretaking is obviously rewarding, because it increases a person’s genetic fitness. However, this reward is rather implicit, in contrast to direct and immediate rewards associated with baby body odor (Lundstrom et al. 2013). Perceiving babies’ body odor as pleasant may thus be one of the mechanisms contributing to cost compensation and creation of familial bonds despite all problems associated with initial caretaking.

The parent-child relationship is affected and shaped by a multiplicity of changes during the development of the child, e.g., social or cognitive development. With increasing self-dependence, it is no longer pivotal that the parents always prioritize their child. Hence, the biological triggers of care, such as the baby schema (Kindchenschema) (Glocker et al. 2009), lose importance and vanish during transition into adulthood (Luo et al. 2011; Volk et al. 2007). We assume that the same is true for the child’s body odor. Anecdotal observations support this: While parents seem to enjoy the body odor of their babies, they rarely talk with the same fascination about the body odor of their pubertal or postpubertal children.

Body odors exhibit a considerable amount of variance, related to stable and variable compounds (Fialová et al. 2016; Leyden et al. 1981; Penn et al. 2007; Roberts et al. 2005; Schaefer et al. 2001). In a group of elements potentially important in the context of bonding, one can mention at least two different systems influencing body odor: peptides from the stable and genetically determined HLA complex (Milinski et al. 2013) and compounds related to development-dependent phenotypic traits (Thornhill et al. 2003). These developmental compounds change as a function of age and are related to the natural hormonal changes taking place as a child gets older (Blakemore et al. 2010).

It is thought that genetically determined body odor compounds (resulting from the genetic similarity of children and their parents) foster initial bonding. The sense of smell is known to be involved in kin recognition in animals (e.g., hamsters (Todrank et al. 1998)) and in humans (Havlicek and Roberts 2009). Kin recognition fulfills two critical biological functions. First, it facilitates attachment to family members which in turn increases investment in genetically related individuals. Accordingly, altruistic tendencies toward genetic relatives are higher than those directed toward unrelated individuals in many animal species including humans (Burnstein et al. 1994; Chapais et al. 2001). Second, kin recognition facilitates inbreeding avoidance, wherein inbreeding is associated with reduced fitness of resultant offspring (Park et al. 2008); mating of genetically similar partners may lead to inbreeding depression, i.e., higher rates of dangerous mutations (Charlesworth and Charlesworth 1987). In support of this, it has been found that couples who cohabitated early in childhood have an increased sexual aversion toward one another (Wolf 1995). It is thus possible that kin recognition depends not only on genetic factors, but that it can also be learned through exposure during childhood.

The interaction between genetic and developmental factors during the course of a child’s growth might serve as an incest-avoidance barrier. Two existing studies on this issue (Ferdenzi et al. 2010; Weisfeld et al. 2003) report contradictory results on whether parents of pubertal children find the odors of their children pleasant. Weisfeld and collaborators (Weisfeld et al. 2003) found that body odors of pubertal children were perceived as unpleasant by the parents. However, Ferdenzi et al. (2010) did not observe this effect. Both of these studies have the strong advantage of utilizing real body odor samples. However, because it is difficult to investigate body odor perception within families, these studies were conducted in small sample sizes of 24 (Weisfeld et al. 2003), and 18 children (Ferdenzi et al. 2010) in total. Unfortunately, this prevents us to draw definite conclusions about age-related changes in body odor pleasantness, and more data are clearly warranted. We used a less costly method using a questionnaire on a larger sample of 367 children; we tested whether the perceived pleasantness of a child’s personal odor decreases as a function of age. In addition, we tested whether a potential drop in personal odor pleasantness affected opposite-sex parent-child relationships more than same-sex parent child relationships. Different patterns of changes in pleasantness for same/opposite-sex parents would address the hypotheses concerning inbreeding avoidance. Generally, potential incest might be problematic in relationships with opposite-sex parents. Therefore, if smell-based inbreeding avoidance was observed, personal odor of own pubertal and postpubertal children should be less pleasant for opposite-sex parents than for same-sex parents.

Method

Participants

Our study originally comprised 283 parents. None of those parents had shared offspring, so either father or mother of a family was included in the study. All of them spokePolish. The participating parents were recruited from various university campuses acrossWroclaw and Brzeg (Poland). They were students, participants of vocational courses, participants of local sports events organized at the campuses, participants of evening courses for adults, and passersby (the campuses are located close to the city center). They were approached by the experimenters and trained research assistants and asked to complete a short questionnaire. The paper-and-pencil questionnaires were then individually completed by people who declared having at least one child. Participants were asked (but not formally tested) whether they experience any olfactory problems (i.e., whether they assess their smell as poor or if they had any chronic diseases associated with nose or sinuses). Three parents declared to have problems with their sense of smell and were excluded from the final sample. We also excluded all parents who declared that they were unable to recognize the body odor of their children (n=20), nonbiological parents (n=10), and people who declared that they spent less than 10min per day with their children (n=7).

The final sample included 235 parents (163 mothers and 72 fathers), aged between 21 and 65years (M=36.9years, SD=7.3). Of these, 28 were single parents, 206 were in a relationship, and one did not indicate an answer to this question. The participants were parents to 367 children aged from 1month to 35years (M=9.3years, SD=6.4). Of these, 184 were girls and 183 were boys. On average, the parents were 27.0±5.2years older than their first child. There was a small negative correlation between the children’s age and the parent-child age difference (r=−0.22, p=0.001), reflecting the current sociodemographic trend of having children at an increasingly older age.

For the purpose of further analyses, the children were divided into four age groups according to the main stages of hormonal development (Dorn et al. 2006): less than 4years (infants), 4 to 8years (prepubertal), 9 to 14years (pubertal), and more than 14years old (postpubertal). Grouping was based on age, not on the individual development of the children. Table Table11 presents descriptive statistics for each age group.

Table 1

Descriptive statistics for each of the age groups of children

Age category
<4years
N=79
4 to 8years
N=114
9 to 14years
N=68
>14years
N=105
MSDnMSDnMSDnMSDn
Age (years)1.91.05.81.410.61.417.83.5
Parent-child relationship18.61.718.51.617.82.517.02.9
Personal odor pleasantness18.22.417.42.816.63.515.24.0
Sex of childFemale39563157
Male40583748

All subjects provided written informed consent prior to their inclusion in the study. The study was approved by the ethical board of the Institute of Psychology (University of Wroclaw, Poland), and it was carried out in accordance with the provisions of the World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki.

Procedure

The parents completed a questionnaire containing questions related to their current partner and children. In the first part of the questionnaire, the participants assessed their relationship with a current partner (visual analogue scale with anchors “very bad” and “very good” as extremes and “neutral” in the middle). They also declared whether they knew their partner’s natural body odor, and when they did, they rated the pleasantness of this odor on a visual analogue scale with anchors “very unpleasant” and “very pleasant” as extremes and neutral in the middle (1.8% who did not know the odor of their partner, and 12.4% who did not answer this question were excluded from the analyses). In the second part of the questionnaire, the parents declared how old their “Child 1” was and marked the child’s sex and whether the child was their biological child. Further, the parents were asked “How much time do you spend with the child during an average working day?” and “How much time do you spend with the child during an average weekend day?” (several hours per day, about 1h per day, about 10min per day, less than 10min per day). Finally, similar to the assessments regarding their partner, the parents assessed their relationship with the child and they rated the pleasantness of the child’s body odor. When the participant had more than one child, he or she was given separate questionnaire sheets for each consecutive child. The answers were coded on a 0–20-point scale, with higher numbers corresponding to a relatively better reported relationship and to higher personal odor pleasantness.

Statistical Methods

Data were analyzed using SPSS 21 in several steps. Data inspection showed that one parent rated the body of the child by far less pleasant than the rest of the parents (about 4 SDs below the group mean). Robust covariance was chosen in the further model to deal with this potential outlier.

First, single linear regression analyses were used to identify potential confounds in parents’ ratings of personal odor pleasantness. Sex of the children and parents, time spent together on an average working day, and satisfaction with the relationship with the child were used as predictors for the target (rating of child personal odor pleasantness). Results showed that satisfaction with the relationship (F[1,365]=49.1, p<0.001; β=0.38) and time spent together (F[1,365]=9.6, p<0.00; β=0.25) significantly impacted the personal odor rating. There were no other significant effects. As both variables correlated with the age of the child (relationship satisfaction: r=−0.29, p<0.001; time spent together: r=−0.25, p<0.001), they were added as confounders for further analysis.

Second, the data were analyzed using a generalized mixed model. Each parent served as subject, and multiple children per parent were treated as repeated measurements. The following fixed main effects were included: age of the child as continuous variable (not grouped), sex of the parent, sex of the child, and the interaction sex of parent by sex of child. Time spent together and overall relationship satisfaction were included as random effects. Robust estimation of covariance was used for calculation. For further analysis and purpose of visualization, children were grouped according to age (see Table Table11).

To investigate the interaction between parent’s and child’s sex (potential effect of incest avoidance), the rating data were grouped by same-sex (mother-daughter; father-son) and opposite-sex (mother-son, father-daughter), and were analyzed using the generalized mixed model approach with same-opposite sex and age group as main factors and age group by same-opposite sex as an interaction factor. Time spent together and overall parent-child relationship satisfaction were included as random effects. Robust estimation of covariance was used for calculation. For both generalized mixed model analyses, we also checked the impact of the order of children. For this purpose, we included another random effect in the analysis which indicated for each child whether it was the first, second, or third child in a given family. This did not change the results.

For the purpose of data visualization, personal odor ratings were grouped into the following categories: very unpleasant (0–4), unpleasant (4–8), neutral (8–12), pleasant (12–16), and very pleasant (16–20).

Results

Age of the child significantly impacted on ratings of personal odor pleasantness (F[1,361]=16.2, p<0.001). Ratings of personal odor pleasantness decreased as a function of the child’s age (compare Table Table11 and Fig. Fig.1).1). Sex of the parents did not contribute significantly (F[1,361]=0.49, p=0.49), and there was no significant effect of sex of child (F[1,361]=0.02, p=0.89) or an interaction between sex of parent and child (F[1,361]=0.4, p=0.53) on the perceived personal odor pleasantness (compare Table Table22 and Fig. Fig.1).1). The same pattern of results was found when the data were grouped into age categories. Thus, age category significantly affected ratings of personal odor pleasantness (F[3,359]=7.8, p<0.001) and perceived personal odor pleasantness decreased linearly as a function of a child’s age (Fig. (Fig.1).1). There was no effect of the sex of the parent (F[1,359]=0.59, p=0.44) or child (F[1,359]=0.06, p=0.81) and no interaction between sex of parent and child (F[1,359]=0.37, p=0.54).

Babies Smell Wonderful to Their Parents, Teenagers Do Not: an Exploratory Questionnaire Study on Children’s Age and Personal Odor Ratings in a Polish Sample (3)

a Personal odor pleasantness in relation to the age of the child. A significant decrease was observed as a function of children’s age. Personal odor ratings for the oldest children were approximately in the same range as those for partners (b). Ratings for same-sex and opposite-sex children follow a similar pattern (c). For visualization purposes, all personal odor ratings were grouped into the following categories: very unpleasant (0–4), unpleasant (4–8), neutral (8–12), pleasant (12–16), and very pleasant (16–20)

Table 2

Personal odor pleasantness rating according to sex of the parent and child

Sex of children
GirlsBoys
Age of childrenAge of children
<4years4 to 8years9 to 14years>14years<4years4 to 8years9 to 14years>14years
MSDnMSDnMSDnMSDnMSDnMSDnMSDnMSDn
Sex of parentsMothers18.62.22517.32.84316.53.32415.74.43417.92.52917.82.33717.23.22514.63.835
Fathers18.41.61416.83.61316.44.2815.73.82317.63.71117.23.42115.63.91214.33.913

Planned post hoc tests revealed that perceived personal odor pleasantness was significantly higher for infants than for pubertal (t=2.5, p=0.02) and postpubertal (t=4.6, p<0.001) children. Figure Figure11 shows that for infants, 93.7% of the parents rated their child’s personal odor as pleasant or very pleasant; for pubertal children, this proportion dropped to 83.8%, and for postpubertal children, only 75.2% of parents rated the personal odor of their child as a pleasant sensation. This latter value matches the average rating given for a partner’s personal odor, which was rated as pleasant or very pleasant by 76.7% of the participants.

As indicated in the “Statistical Methods” section, we decided to investigate the interaction between parent’s and child’s sex to observe whether there was a potential effect of incest avoidance in our sample. There was no significant overall effect of same-opposite sex (F[1,358]=0.4; p=0.5), and importantly, there was no significant interaction between same-opposite sex and age group (F[1,358]=0.5, p=0.7).

Discussion

In line with both, our prediction and previous findings (Ferdenzi et al. 2010; Fleming et al. 1993), our study showed that parents very much like the personal odor of their young children. However, the older the child, the more the personal odor ratings of parents dropped. Personal odor of postpubertal children, although still rated as positive (above neutral), was significantly less pleasant than that of infants.

It is possible that developmental changes in children’s hormone levels modulate parents’ perceptions of their personal odors. Increases in sex hormone levels around the time of puberty may modulate personal odors in such a way that they are rated less favorably by parents. Consequently, the initially very high pleasantness ratings of a child’s personal odor decrease in postpubertal children to approximately the level of how personal odors of other beloved people (e.g., the partner) are rated.

In principle, two main stages of hormonal development can be distinguished (Dorn et al. 2006): adrenarche and gonadarche. Adrenarche, or activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, typically begins between the age of 6 and 9 in girls and a year later in boys. Adrenal androgens begin to rise and contribute to the development of secondary sexual characteristics such as axillary and pubic hair and changes in body odor. Gonadarche begins with activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-gonadal (HPG) axis and ends with the attainment of fecundity. The maturing ovaries and testes also secrete the gonadal steroids estrogen and testosterone, respectively. Derivates of those hormones change body odor throughout a child’s development (Chopra et al. 2008). This process terminates around the age of 8 to 14years in girls and 9 to 15 in boys. Interestingly, the earlier onset of developmental hormones in girls was reflected in our data (see Fig. Fig.1c),1c), although not statistically significant.

We did not find support for a potential incest avoidance effect on perceived odor pleasantness, as the effect of enhanced body odor aversions in opposite-sex parent-child relations (Weisfeld et al. 2003) could not be confirmed. In line with previous suggestions (Lieberman et al. 2003), it seems that olfactory recognition mechanisms are far more likely to support close parent-child bonding before sexual maturity than inbreeding avoidance after maturity. However, further studies in this area are necessary before any definite conclusions can be made.

We are aware of several limitations of the study. First, personal odor pleasantness was retrieved from memory. Although we controlled for potential biases, such as reduced olfactory function and/or memory of the child’s personal odor, other potential confounds such as sinunasal diseases could not be entirely eliminated. Also, the reports of perceived pleasantness could be driven by other factors than a child’s natural body odor. Various cultural practices contribute to the final personal odor (Allen et al. 2015), and we can expect that their influence becomes stronger with age as pubescents/adolescents begin to smoke, drink, wear perfume, etc., hence differentially affecting parental reports across the age categories (Martinec Nováková et al. 2017). Further, parents’ ratings were not blind to the identity of a rated person and as a result could be superimposed by a general evaluation of the child, which is also reflected in the rather high correlation between odor pleasantness and relationship satisfaction. Although we controlled for parent-child relationship ratings in our analyses, we cannot exclude the possibility that some results were partly due to a so-called “halo” effect (Nisbett and Wilson 1977). Especially as our data were collected via questionnaires and not in an experimental setting, such effects are possible.

Our sample was based on volunteers; therefore, a participation bias may occur. It has for instance been shown that volunteers are more likely to be female, highly educated. and extraverted compared to nonvolunteers of experimental studies (Bortz and Döring 2013). Because our entire sample was recruited in one country, we cannot generalize for different countries or cultures. Finally, subtle effects such as potential incest avoidance are unlikely to be fully reflected in our data and might be better assessed using a blinded procedure of body odor assessment.

Despite these limitations, our study provides novel support for a hypothesis that body odor perception plays a potentially strong role in parent-child relationship. It further suggests that research involving children’s personal odors should not collapse across age, but include age or at least age group as suggested in our study, as a moderating factor.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Katarzyna Pisanski for language help. The study was supported by a grant from the German Research Foundation to IC (CR 479/4-1). During the project, AS was supported by Polish Ministry of Science and Higher Education (scholarship for years 2013–2016). The funding sources had no involvement in study design; in the collection, analysis, and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; and in the decision to submit the article for publication.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Contributor Information

Ilona Croy, Email: ed.nedserd-ut@yorc.anoli.

Tomasz Frackowiak, Email: lp.ude.rwu@kaiwokcarf.zsamot.

Thomas Hummel, Email: ed.nedserd-ut.hiz.liam@lemmuht.

Agnieszka Sorokowska, Email: moc.liamg@akswokoroS.

References

  • Allen C, Havlíček J, Roberts SC. Effect of fragrance use on discrimination of individual body odor. Front Psychol. 2015;6:1115. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Blakemore SJ, Burnett S, Dahl RE. The role of puberty in the developing adolescent brain. Hum Brain Mapp. 2010;31(6):926–933. doi:10.1002/hbm.21052. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Bojanowski V, Hummel T, Croy I. Isolated congenital anosmia—clinical and daily life aspects of a life without a sense of smell. Laryngo-Rhino-Otologie. 2013;92(1):30–33. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bortz J, Döring N (2013) Forschungsmethoden und evaluation. Springer-Verlag
  • Bowlby J (1988) Attachment, communication, and the therapeutic process. A secure base: parent-child attachment and healthy human development 137–157
  • Burnstein E, Crandall C, Kitayama S. Some neo-Darwinian decision rules for altruism: weighing cues for inclusive fitness as a function of the biological importance of the decision. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1994;67(5):773. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.5.773. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Chapais B, Savard L, Gauthier C. Kin selection and the distribution of altruism in relation to degree of kinship in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata) Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2001;49(6):493–502. doi:10.1007/s002650100335. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Charlesworth D, Charlesworth B (1987) Inbreeding depression and its evolutionary consequences. Annual review of ecology and systematics 237–268
  • Chopra A, Baur A, Hummel T. Thresholds and chemosensory event-related potentials to malodors before, during, and after puberty: differences related to sex and age. NeuroImage. 2008;40(3):1257–1263. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.01.015. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Corter CM, Fleming AS (1995) Psychobiology of maternal behavior in human beings
  • Del Cerro MCR. Role of the vomeronasal input in maternal behavior. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1998;23(8):905–926. doi:10.1016/S0306-4530(98)00060-2. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Depue RA, Morrone-Strupinsky JV. A neurobehavioral model of affiliative bonding: implications for conceptualizing a human trait of affiliation. Behav Brain Sci. 2005;28(3):313–349. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Dorn LD, Dahl RE, Woodward HR, Biro F. Defining the boundaries of early adolescence: a user’s guide to assessing pubertal status and pubertal timing in research with adolescents. Appl Dev Sci. 2006;10(1):30–56. doi:10.1207/s1532480xads1001_3. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Ferdenzi C, Schaal B, Roberts SC. Family scents: developmental changes in the perception of kin body odor? J Chem Ecol. 2010;36(8):847–854. doi:10.1007/s10886-010-9827-x. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Fialová J, Roberts SC, Havlíček J. Consumption of garlic positively affects hedonic perception of axillary body odour. Appetite. 2016;97:8–15. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2015.11.001. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Fleming AS, Corter C, Franks P, Surbey M, Schneider B, Steiner M. Postpartum factors related to mother’s attraction to newborn infant odors. Dev Psychobiol. 1993;26(2):115–132. doi:10.1002/dev.420260204. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Glocker ML, Langleben DD, Ruparel K, Loughead JW, Gur RC, Sachser N. Baby schema in infant faces induces cuteness perception and motivation for caretaking in adults. Ethology. 2009;115(3):257–263. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.2008.01603.x. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Havlicek J, Roberts SC. MHC-correlated mate choice in humans: a review. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34(4):497–512. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.10.007. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Kaitz M, Good A, Rokem A, Eidelman A. Mothers’ recognition of their newborns by olfactory cues. Dev Psychobiol. 1987;20(6):587–591. doi:10.1002/dev.420200604. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Leckman JF, Mayes LC, Feldman R, Evans DW, King RA, Cohen DJ. Early parental preoccupations and behaviors and their possible relationship to the symptoms of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1999;100(S396):1–26. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.1999.tb10951.x. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Leyden JJ, McGinley KJ, Hölzle E, Labows JN, Kligman AM. The microbiology of the human axilla and its relationship to axillary odor. J Investig Dermatol. 1981;77(5):413–416. doi:10.1111/1523-1747.ep12494624. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Lieberman D, Tooby J, Cosmides L (2003) The evolution of human incest avoidance mechanisms: an evolutionary psychological approach. Evolution and the moral emotions: appreciating Edward Westermarc
  • Lundstrom JN, Mathe A, Schaal B, Frasnelli J, Nitzsche K, Gerber J, Hummel T. Maternal status regulates cortical responses to the body odor of newborns. Front Psychol. 2013;4:597. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00597. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Luo LZ, Li H, Lee K. Are children’s faces really more appealing than those of adults? Testing the baby schema hypothesis beyond infancy. J Exp Child Psychol. 2011;110(1):115–124. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.04.002. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Martinec Nováková L, Plotěná D, Havlíček J (2017) Age and pubertal status-related changes in reports of perception of personal odors. Perception 0301006616686096 [PubMed]
  • Milinski M, Croy I, Hummel T, Boehm T. Major histocompatibility complex peptide ligands as olfactory cues in human body odour assessment. Proc Biol Sci. 2013;280(1755):20122889. doi:10.1098/rspb.2012.2889. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Nisbett RE, Wilson TD. The halo effect: evidence for unconscious alteration of judgments. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1977;35(4):250. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.35.4.250. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Park JH, Schaller M, Van Vugt M. Psychology of human kin recognition: heuristic cues, erroneous inferences, and their implications. Rev Gen Psychol. 2008;12(3):215. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.12.3.215. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Penn DJ, Oberzaucher E, Grammer K, Fischer G, Soini HA, Wiesler D, et al. Individual and gender fingerprints in human body odour. J R Soc Interface. 2007;4(13):331–340. doi:10.1098/rsif.2006.0182. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Porter RH, Cernoch JM, McLaughlin FJ. Maternal recognition of neonates through olfactory cues. Physiol Behav. 1983;30(1):151–154. doi:10.1016/0031-9384(83)90051-3. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Roberts SC, Gosling LM, Spector TD, Miller P, Penn DJ, Petrie M. Body odor similarity in noncohabiting twins. Chem Senses. 2005;30(8):651–656. doi:10.1093/chemse/bji058. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Schaal B, Montagner H, Hertling E, Bolzoni D, Moyse A, Quichon R. Les stimulations olfactives dans les relations entre l’enfant et la mere. Reprod Nutr Dev. 1980;20(3B):843–858. doi:10.1051/rnd:19800510. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Schaefer ML, Young DA, Restrepo D. Olfactory fingerprints for major histocompatibility complex-determined body odors. J Neurosci. 2001;21(7):2481–2487. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Thornhill R, Gangestad SW, Miller R, Scheyd G, McCollough JK, Franklin M. Major histocompatibility complex genes, symmetry, and body scent attractiveness in men and women. Behav Ecol. 2003;14(5):668–678. doi:10.1093/beheco/arg043. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Todrank J, Heth G, Johnston RE. Kin recognition in golden hamsters: evidence for kinship odours. Anim Behav. 1998;55(2):377–386. doi:10.1006/anbe.1997.0611. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Volk AA, Lukjanczuk JL, Quinsey VL. Perceptions of child facial cues as a function of child age. Evol Psychol. 2007;5(4):147470490700500409. doi:10.1177/147470490700500409. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Weisfeld GE, Czilli T, Phillips KA, Gall JA, Lichtman CM. Possible olfaction-based mechanisms in human kin recognition and inbreeding avoidance. J Exp Child Psychol. 2003;85(3):279–295. doi:10.1016/S0022-0965(03)00061-4. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Wolf AP (1995) Sexual attraction and childhood association: a Chinese brief for Edward Westermarck. Stanford University Press
Babies Smell Wonderful to Their Parents, Teenagers Do Not: an Exploratory Questionnaire Study on Children’s Age and Personal Odor Ratings in a Polish Sample (2024)

FAQs

Do babies have better sense of smell than adults? ›

Newborns outperform adults at a variety of smell tests. For example, newborns detect odor components in human sweat better than adults do. Other research has shown that babies recognize the smell of amniotic fluid and prefer to nurse from a breast that has been moistened with their own amniotic fluid.

Why do people think babies smell good? ›

In addition to being an identifying feature for their parents, a 2013 study showed the scent of a baby triggered women's dopamine pathways in the region of the brain associated with rewards. These are the same pathways that are triggered by things like the smell of delicious food or satisfying a food craving.

What is the importance of smell in social analysis? ›

Beyond food enjoyment, as part of quality of life, smell has the ability to transfer and regulate emotional conditions, and thus impacts social relationships, at various stages across life (e.g., prenatal and postnatal, during puberty, for partner selection and in sickness).

Can babies smell their parents? ›

Babies recognize their mother's scent even before they are born. Your baby is biologically and genetically programmed to connect to you through your unique smell. The process of development of olfactory cells (cells responsible for the sense of smell) begins as soon as the first trimester of pregnancy.

Why do I always want to smell my baby? ›

It turns out that the fact you want to gobble up your sweetie isn't your fault—it's biology. Scientists have recently learned that in new mothers, the body odour of newborns activates the centres of the brain that perceive rewards and pleasure—and makes those moms hungry for more.

Do younger people have a better sense of smell? ›

Sense of smell improves up to about the age of eight. But from the age of 20 (or even 15, according to some studies), the sense gently declines.

Does it smell when you give birth? ›

What is lochia? Lochia is the vagin*l discharge you have after giving birth. It contains a mix of blood, mucus and uterine tissue. It has a stale, musty odor like menstrual period discharge and can last several weeks.

Why do babies headbutt? ›

The reasons

“This is similar to any tantrum,” Schreck says. “It's usually brought on by frustration or lack of attention.” Typically manifesting between 18 months and 2 years old, the behavior is a way for toddlers to send a message to the parent. It should subside once they start talking at around 3 or 4.

Why does my child smell so good to me? ›

There's limited research on the subject, but many experts theorize that the "what" behind the newborn scent is the vernix caseosa. That's the white, waxy coating that protects your baby's skin in utero and holds in moisture. (Think of it as natural Vaseline.)

Why smell is so important in psychology? ›

The sense of smell is closely linked with memory, probably more so than any of our other senses. Those with full olfactory function may be able to think of smells that evoke particular memories; the scent of an orchard in blossom conjuring up recollections of a childhood picnic, for example.

How does smell affect human behavior? ›

A number of studies have shown that the odors people like make them feel good, whereas odors people dislike make them feel bad. These mood responses have also been reported physiologically.

How do smells affect people's moods? ›

Many fragrances possess strong associative properties, and work easily to alter the mood, promoting increased alertness and positivity, or creating feelings of calm, tranquillity and relaxation. They can be helpful in alleviating stress, easing insomnia, providing clarity and focus.

How long can your baby smell you? ›

Although the newborn scent eventually fades (usually around six weeks of age) many parents (including this one) are known to take a deep whiff of their child's head (regardless of their age) and savor the unique scent and comfort it provides you.

At what age can babies smell? ›

By 10 weeks, the receptors that your baby will use to detect smells have already formed. Babies start using their sense of smell while still in the womb. As they breathe and swallow amniotic fluid, they become familiar with its scent.

Does a baby know its father? ›

When do babies recognize their father or mother? Babies can recognize their parents pretty early actually – as young as 4 days old. By making eye contact with your baby during feeding times, cuddle sessions and throughout the day, you're helping your child memorize your face and learn to trust you.

Why do babies smile in their sleep? ›

Rapid Eye Movement (REM) or Active Sleep

The vast majority of newborns' smiles during sleep appear to occur when the eyes are moving rapidly, as they would during a dream. Studies suggest that adults smile in response to positive dream imagery.

What do dogs think of new babies? ›

Canines are smitten with babies and can form strong connections because of the amount of time spent together. Both a baby and a dog, especially a young pup, have a common desire for a playmate and someone who will give them attention.

Do strong smells affect babies? ›

Your baby is getting used to different smells, but can still find strong aromas overpowering. In fact, a strong smell can interfere with his sense of taste. If you're wearing a strong perfume, he may not feed as well as usual . Your baby is becoming more aware of different people around him.

Does your smell get worse as you age? ›

Your sense of smell can also diminish, especially after age 70. This may be related to a loss of nerve endings and less mucus production in the nose. Mucus helps odors stay in the nose long enough to be detected by the nerve endings. It also helps clear odors from the nerve endings.

Does everyone have a special scent? ›

Every person has a unique scent. “It's like a fingerprint,” says Johan Lundström, a neuroscientist at the Karolinska Institute in Sweden. “There is a large genetic component to body odor. Even trained sniffer dogs have a hard time distinguishing between identical twins, unless the twins are on different diets.”

Why does smell get worse as we get older? ›

Some loss of taste and smell is natural with aging, especially after age 60. But other factors can contribute to loss of taste and smell, including: Nasal and sinus problems, such as allergies, sinusitis or nasal polyps. Viral infections, including the common cold and the flu.

What does childbirth feel like? ›

Some people describe the feeling as being like intense period cramps, others say it feels like a tightening or pounding feeling in your uterus or across your belly, others describe the feeling as being like very intense muscle cramps, while still other people describe contractions as being like the sort of wrenching ...

Why do babies slap you? ›

While these can sometimes be due to frustration, often they are driven by sensory exploration. Some children like the feel of slapping and the sensory feedback they get.

Why should you not touch a baby's head? ›

A baby's soft spots are called fontanelles. They allow your baby's brain to grow larger at a fast rate over their first year of life. It's important to avoid pressing into their soft spots, as it could cause damage to their skull or brain.

Why do babies shake their head before latching? ›

Shaking head when nursing

One of the first times babies shake their heads is when they nurse from their mothers. This may first occur out of your baby's attempt to try to latch. As your baby gets the hang of latching on, the shaking may then be a result of excitement.

Why do moms like to smell their kids? ›

“Many parents report that their baby's odor smells pleasant, rewarding and adorable,” Schäfer said. “This suggests infantile body odors can mediate affectionate love towards the child in the crucial periods of bonding.

Can babies smell their fathers? ›

Right from birth, a baby can recognize their parent's voice and smell, says Dr. Laible. The next step is linking those sounds and smells with something they can see. That's why they'll start studying your face as if they're trying to memorize it.

Do children have a strong sense of smell? ›

Generally, olfaction in children seems to be very good, including sensitivity to body odors [23–26]. Many studies have shown that children can detect, discriminate, and respond to odors and that they can do it starting from the very beginning of their lives.

What does it mean when you smell someone that isn't there? ›

An olfactory hallucination (phantosmia) makes you detect smells that aren't really there in your environment. The odors you notice in phantosmia are different from person to person and may be foul or pleasant. You may notice the smells in one or both nostrils.

Why does smell trigger memory? ›

Scents bypass the thalamus and go straight to the brain's smell center, known as the olfactory bulb. The olfactory bulb is directly connected to the amygdala and hippocampus, which might explain why the smell of something can so immediately trigger a detailed memory or even intense emotion.

What does it mean when a guy sniffs your hair? ›

When a guy smells your hair, it could mean that he's attracted to you and is trying to get a better sense of your scent. It could mean that your hair smells great and he's enjoying the smell. What is this? It could also be a form of flirting (watch his body language) or an attempt to start a conversation.

Can humans smell anger? ›

Do they smell too? Glad you asked, A new study suggests anger does indeed have a scent. German and Swedish researchers had 15 healthy male volunteers give a sample of their body odor, once after a boxing session and once after using an exercise machine.

Can guys smell when a girl is on her period? ›

And what about during your period? Previous studies have shown that a woman's body odor is strongest during menstruation and that men who are particularly sensitive to smells can even detect this change in her scent.

Can your partner make you smell? ›

Anything that gets in there can alter your pH levels and affect how you smell. If you have P-in-V sex, sem*n — which is alkaline and the opposite of your vagin*'s acidic environment — can temporarily change your smell.

What smells make people angry? ›

Cigarette smoke and polluted air have long been known to increase aggressive behavior, while rose odor is known to decrease aggressive behavior, raising the possibility of rose scent as an effective strategy in anger management.

What smell makes people happiest? ›

Scents to make you happy
  1. Lavender. The aromatherapy world's queen jewel! ...
  2. Jasmine. Those tiny white jasmine flowers have been making humans happy for millennia! ...
  3. Neroli. Neroli is like a breath of sweet citrus air! ...
  4. Rose. ...
  5. Yuzu.

Does being happy make you smell better? ›

But new research suggests there may be a less obvious way to pick up on another person's positive vibes: smell. According to a team of European researchers, happiness may generate chemicals that get secreted in sweat, and that sweat signal gets sniffed by those around us.

Do babies sleep better with moms scent? ›

By incorporating a parent's scent, the baby feels as though you are there physically with them, even when you are not. A parent's scent alone has the ability to reduce cortisol levels in babies, in turn reducing stress and anxiety, helping to promote longer, more sound sleep (for both the baby and parent).

Why does my baby smell sour? ›

Your baby may also occasionally have a sour smell if he's spit up or has sweat or dirt trapped somewhere on his body. "Young kids have a short neck and lots of fat, so food and sweat can be trapped in the folds of skin and start to have an odor," Mazur says. In this case, a bath should get rid of the smell.

Can a baby see at birth? ›

At birth, a newborn's eyesight is between 20/200 and 20/400. Their eyes are sensitive to bright light, so they're more likely to open their eyes in low light. Don't worry if your baby's eyes sometimes cross or drift outward (go "wall-eyed"). This is normal until your baby's vision improves and eye muscles strengthen.

Can a 2 month old smell? ›

Experiments suggest that newborns are actually better at detecting the odor components in human sweat than adults are (Loos et al 2017). Other research shows that newborn babies can recognize the smell of amniotic fluid (Varendi et al 1997). They can also distinguish between the scent of breast milk and formula.

What age does a baby smile at you? ›

Typically, babies start smiling between 6 and 12 weeks, but you may notice a smile or smirk soon after baby's born.

What color does baby see first? ›

Young babies are indeed capable of seeing colors, but their brains may not perceive them as clearly or vividly as older children and adults do. The first primary color your baby can see is red, and this happens a few weeks into life.

What age is the sense of smell fully developed? ›

Your baby's sense of smell will continue to develop until he is around eight years old .

Do babies have a strong sense of smell? ›

The brain's olfactory (smell) center forms very early in fetal development. Studies have found that newborns have a keen sense of smell. Within the first few days they will show a preference for the smell of their own mother, especially to her breast milk. Touch.

Do babies know their fathers scent? ›

Right from birth, a baby can recognize their parent's voice and smell, says Dr. Laible. The next step is linking those sounds and smells with something they can see. That's why they'll start studying your face as if they're trying to memorize it.

Top Articles
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Prof. Nancy Dach

Last Updated:

Views: 6193

Rating: 4.7 / 5 (77 voted)

Reviews: 84% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Prof. Nancy Dach

Birthday: 1993-08-23

Address: 569 Waelchi Ports, South Blainebury, LA 11589

Phone: +9958996486049

Job: Sales Manager

Hobby: Web surfing, Scuba diving, Mountaineering, Writing, Sailing, Dance, Blacksmithing

Introduction: My name is Prof. Nancy Dach, I am a lively, joyous, courageous, lovely, tender, charming, open person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.